The current research sought to understand the contribution of novelty to the positive outcomes that have been reported in the research on 3D interactions online and in e-commerce. The novelty of 3D product presentations was manipulated and examined along with the presentation of additional forms of information to determine if novelty is related to positive feelings associated with 3D products. Additional information was most compelling in product attitude formation, whereas novelty played a key role in shaping purchase intention. Novelty and information both impacted attitudes toward Web sites using 3D products. Directions for future research are suggested.
Estimates indicate rapid growth
in e-tailing sales and total online spending in the US. According
to David J. Roddy, chief telecommunications economist at Deloitte
Research, Internet revenues will surpass $1.1 trillion by 2002,
more than 70 percent of large companies will start using the Web
as a sales medium, and time and distance will no longer be an
obstacle between buyers and sellers. This 24- hour global purchase
option is something that is going to drastically change the way
people shop and spend. Jupiter Communications (1999) estimates
U.S. e-tailing sales to exceed $41.1 billion in 2002. Although
various researchers and organizations differ in their estimates,
they all agree on the fact that e-commerce is a rapidly developing
field and that the world is to witness major changes in the way
people communicate and trade over the Internet.
With the increase in online traffic and sales, many e-commerce
merchants are turning to 3D product presentations to stand out
in such a competitive environment and to provide consumers with
greater sensory experience (Nash 2000). This makes it important
to understand the impact of virtual experiences. To date, researchers
have examined the relative advantages of 3D presentations over
direct and indirect media as well as consumer learning in 3D environments
(Li, Daugherty and Biocca 2001a, Li, Daugherty and Biocca 2001b).
Other research has gone on to measure the effectiveness of interactive
advertising by measuring attitudes, behaviors and cognitive and
affective responses (Pavlou and Stewart 2000). However, the novelty
of interacting in these environments has been overlooked. The
excitement of novel stimuli may result in positive attitude formation,
but what happens when interacting in a 3 dimensional space is
no longer novel? The proposed research seeks to understand the
contribution of novelty to the positive outcomes that have been
reported in early research on 3D interactions online and in e-commerce.
The Internet is fundamentally different from traditional
media in that it provides for two-way interactivity unlike print,
TV and other traditional media (Cho 1999; Hoffman and Novak 1996).
Meeker (1997) described the Internet as the only medium that allows
consumers to see products, investigate further details and immediately
make purchases. Plus, a relatively early study found that the
Internet increases brand awareness and brand image as effectively
as traditional media (Briggs and Hollis 1997). However, the Internet
is not just an efficient channel for advertising, marketing and
product distribution, but the interactivity and multimedia displays
can create a sense of enjoyment which may result in a loss of
self-consciousness (Hoffman and Novak 1996). This loss of self-consciousness
creates a compelling online experience that mirrors reality and
can enhance consumer learning (Novak, Hoffman and Yung 2000).
Interaction with 3D products online is one way to enhance online
experiences and may add to what is known in e-commerce lingo as
‘virtual experience.’
Conceptually, ‘virtual experience’ has been defined
as “psychological and emotional states that consumers undergo
while interacting with products in a 3D environment” (Li,
Daugherty, and Biocca 2001b). Virtual experience may have advantages
that have previously been associated with both direct and indirect
product experiences. Direct experience has been defined as “an
experience that stems out of an unmediated interaction between
the consumer and the product, with a person’s full sensory
capacity, including visual, auditory, taste-smell, haptic and
orienting” (Gibson 1966) and is thought to cause consumers
to have greater confidence in their product choices (Hoch and
Deighton 1989). In contrast, indirect experience stems from
symbolic representations of the world experienced through communications
with others and mediated representations of the world presented
in books, magazines, and television. Indirect experience is believed
to result in less affective responses and thus be less effective
in changing attitudes than direct experience (Millar and Millar
1996). However, providing consumers indirect experiences through
media have traditionally been one of the easiest and cheapest
ways to persuade large audiences with commercial messages. Virtual
experience is beneficial because it has the common factor of interactivity
(Hoch and Deighton 1989) yet it is a mediated experience (Heeter
2000) that can be provided to large audiences. Hence, virtual
experience may be able to enjoy the advantages of both direct
and indirect experiences (Li, Daugherty and Biocca 2001a). In
fact, Li, Daugherty and Biocca (2001b) found that virtual experience
created by 3D environments was much better than indirect experience
created by traditional media in facilitating learning.
Given the above findings, it seems evident that 3D product presentations
may be superior to other mediated forms of experience for facilitating
information processing. The research currently being conducted
seems to be bolstering these results. However, one aspect of interacting
in 3D environments that has yet to be examined is the novelty
of product presentations. People who have not been exposed to
3D representations of products online may simply be more curious
than if the information was presented in another medium or format.
Thus, it is possible that favorable attitudes could result from
the novelty of interacting with 3D products, and not from
the 3D products themselves.
Novel stimuli have been found to stimulate vigorous and active information processing, resulting in better recall and recognition than when non-novel stimuli were used (Fahy, Riches and Brown 1993; Li, Miller and Desimone 1993; Riches, Wilson and Brown 1991; Wilson and Rolls 1993). Pfau, Parrott and Lindquist (1992) described positive effects of novelty, even when the novelty was associated with negative political attack ads. One of the means by which novelty aids information processing is through attention (Kover and James 1993). Attention is thought to be on a continuum from inward to outward focus. According to Carver and Scheier (1981), novelty distracts an individual’s self focus away from an internal state. This decrease in the attention to self results in increased attention to the environment. Increased attention is the first step needed to process information. In fact, Steenkamp and Baumgartner (1992) identified purposeful information seeking and curiosity seeking as two motives that underlie information seeking. A novel 3D product presentation may enhance information seeking behavior and elicit increased curiosity. Therefore, novelty may be a necessary step leading to further processing of information.
Novel stimuli are also thought to elicit and
enhance a person’s exploratory behavior (Spielberger and Starr
1994). Exploratory behaviors are defined by curiosity, variety
seeking, and risk taking (Raju 1980). Curiosity represents a
tendency to seek stimulation from a variety of sources that
are novel, often as a reaction to boredom. Novelty feeds the
need for change, thereby alleviating a state of boredom (Steenkamp
and Baumgartner 1992). Therefore, when faced with novel stimuli
online, one may be more inclined to pay attention, seek out
more information, and possibly engage in impulse purchasing.
Deci (1992) also claimed that novelty is one of seven dimensions
that represent situational interest. According to Reeve (1996,
p.170), “situational interest is a person-activity interactive
construct and flows from a person’s relationship with
a particular activity.” The more interest a person has
in an activity, the more enjoyment he/she garners from the situation.
When people experience situational interest, it often stems
from a somewhat challenging environment, which demands high
attention and an intention to explore. Instant enjoyment usually
results during the person-environment interaction (Chen and
Darst 2001). Novelty increases situational interest and thereby
increases the degree to which people should find a novel activity
enjoyable. To the degree that people enjoy an experience, they
should also form a positive attitude in response. Therefore,
we propose that the novelty of 3D products heightens situational
interest, increases involvement, and results in a favorable
attitude toward the experience.
While 3D product experiences may be inherently more interesting
than product presented in 2D, novelty could serve as an alternative
explanation for the increased interest and effectiveness of
these new presentation formats. If novelty is playing a role
in the formation of consumer attitudes with 3D products online,
then two aspects of the presentation must be considered when
attempting to explain a novelty effect: 1) the novelty of 3D
product presentations, and 2) other sources of information.
First, as the novelty of the presentation method decreases,
consumers should be more likely to focus on the product attributes.
Cox and Locander (1987) found that consumers were focused on
affective reactions when faced with novel stimuli instead of
product relevant information. Fitzgerald (1999) found that highly
novel analogies used in advertisements were perceived to be
too complex for thorough processing of the product message,
indicating that consumers focused on decoding the message but
did not have the capacity to fully integrate the information.
A more moderate level of novelty produced a more complete understanding
of the product information. Therefore, it appears that novelty
distracts from actively processing and questioning product claims
in advertising.
Tellis (1997) reviewed the literature on effective frequency
in advertising and described two consumer responses to novel
stimuli that are repeated overtime. Tellis (1997) claims that
novel ads lead to uncertainty and tension, but as viewers become
familiar and comfortable with ads they develop positive attitudes
through a process called habituation. However, as the number
of exposures increases and viewers become bored with ads, they
develop negative attitudes through a process called tedium.
Figure 1 shows the relationship between habituation and tedium.
Over time, habituation shows positive effects that peak and
then decline, whereas tedium occurs later but simply declines.
The relative weight of the two effects may help to explain positive
outcomes associated with novel stimuli and why consumers may
become increasingly critical of advertising messages as novelty
wanes.
Haugtvedt, Schumann, Schneier, and Warren (1994) examined advertising
repetition and differing methods of varying ads. They distinguished
between cosmetic variation and substantive variation, which
was contrasted with repeated exposure and exposure to a single
ad. Cosmetic variation uses differing peripheral cues (e.g.,
endorsers, colors, type faces, etc.) to maintain novelty, whereas
substantive variation occurs when product relevant messages
are changed (e.g., lower price versus more cleaning power).
Haugtvedt et al. (1994) concluded that substantive variation
encourages product-related thoughts, whereas cosmetic variation
encourages processing of the ad as a whole. Both forms of novelty
resulted in equally positive attitudes toward the ad. However,
subjects who were exposed to substantive variation showed more
positive attitudes toward the product and recalled a greater
number of product attributes. Subjects who were exposed to the
cosmetic variation recalled more features of the advertisement
itself. Similar to cosmetic variation, 3D product presentations
are likely to create a short-lived novelty effect that focuses
consumers’ attention on the experience and not product
relevant information. Over time, substantive variation of product
attribute information may be needed to maintain positive attitudes
as the novelty effect of the 3D presentations fade.
Oliver, Robertson and Mitchell (1993) examined how consumers’
elaborate on information when faced with novel stimuli in general.
They described two responses to advertising which include: imagery
and analysis. Imagery (imagining the use of a product) was found
to be more prevalent in response to novel ads and was positively
related to purchase intention. Analysis was characterized as
semantic and verbal, and was negatively related to the affective
reaction toward the ads. Novel stimuli were processed differently
than familiar stimuli. Cues in advertisements or the experience
of an ad as a whole may be the “information” processed
in imagery responses to novel stimuli, while product attribute
information is the focus of analysis responses to non-novel
stimuli. Similarly, as 3D product presentations become more
common, consumers may rely less on the “sizzle”
and more on the steak. In light of the literature presented
and all other conditions remaining exactly the same, we propose
that:
H1: As the
level of familiarity with 3D presentations increases, positive
attitudes toward a product presented in 3D will decrease.
H2: As
the level of familiarity with 3D presentations increases, favorable
purchase intention of products presented in a 3D environment
will decrease.
H3: As the
level of familiarity with 3D presentations increases, positive
attitudes toward websites containing 3D product presentations
will decrease.
However, when all other conditions are not the same, the proposed novelty effect may cease to exist. For instance, when 3D product presentations are used in conjunction with copy or product attribute descriptions, the additional information may be perceived as more relevant than the novelty of the method of presentation, particularly when a decision is pending. When asked to make a decision, consumers tend to focus on product relevant attributes (Bettman, Johnson and Payne 1991), as consumers are generally motivated toward a desirable goal (Jolibert and Baumgartner 1997). Given the motivation, ability and opportunity to do so, consumers generally seek out information that is most relevant and increase their level of information processing (MacInnis, Moorman and Jaworski 1991). MacInnis et al. summarize findings showing that higher processing levels enhance memory for brand information, result in more stable attitudes, and impact purchase intentions. According to MacInnis and Jaworski (1989), the more motivated a consumer is by a utilitarian need, the more attention is focused on product attribute information. The more motivated a consumer is by an expressive need, the more attention is focused on cues of the advertisement itself. Therefore, when a purchase decision must be made and product attribute information is available, it is expected that consumers will be likely to use such information when forming attitudes and making a decision. However, we would expect that when product information is presented with a novel 3D product presentation, it is likely that the novelty will distract consumers from processing the information conveyed in the copy (Cox and Locander 1987). In other words, in non-novel situations product information is more likely to influence attitudes and purchase decisions than when product information is presented with a novel stimulus. Therefore, it is expected that:
H4: Information will have a greater effect on attitude towards the product, purchase intention, and attitudes toward Web sites with 3D product presentations when the experience is not novel, more so than when novel.
Finally, one other possible confound that impacts the effectiveness of computer-mediated communication is believability. Research has shown the importance of presenting situations that mirror reality to enhance communication effectiveness (Aaker and Stayman 1989, Beltramini 1982, Hoffman 1986). Edwards and La Ferle (2001) found that believability influenced the degree to which people experienced role-taking in response to 3D virtual reality environments. To the degree that a product presentation is not true to life, consumers are likely to question the believability of the product represented. If not deemed believable, the information will be discounted and may be less likely to be used in making the purchase decision. Therefore:
H5: Purchase intention should be positively related to the perceived believability of the product presentation.
the possible importance of novelty in explaining the effectiveness of 3D e-commerce presentations, the research will examine changes in attitude formation as the novelty of 3D presentations decreases. If novelty results in increased interest, then perhaps 3D presentations will become less effective over time. If so, then it is necessary to understand the interaction of novelty, interest, and the processing of commercial experiences to facilitate effective planning of 3D commercial environments over time.
At a Midwestern university,
138 students were recruited and pretested as to their exposure
to 3D product presentations and virtual reality experiences. Subjects
were then randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions
in which they were exposed to 3D products presented online. A
2 x 2 design was used in which novelty and level of information
were manipulated. To manipulate novelty, subjects either viewed
a single Web site at which a small computer desk was depicted
in 3D, or they viewed four 3D products and then the small computer
desk in 3D. The five exposures to 3D product presentations occurred
in a short period of time which Tellis (1997, p. 78) predicted
would lead, “to tedium, lower interest and attention without
much increase in habituation.” Level of information was
manipulated by exposing subjects to 3D products with no other
information, or 3D products that were accompanied by copy describing
product attribute information (i.e., price, material, color, size,
etc.). Subjects were told that they could spend as much time as
they wished with each depicted product.
Once finished, subjects were asked to fill out a questionnaire
that assessed: 1) attitude toward the computer desk, 2) likelihood
of purchase, 3) attitude toward Websites using such presentations
in general, and 4) intention to view 3D product presentations
in the future. Intention to view 3D product presentations in the
future was measured to determine the degree to which subjects
would like to view 3D products again. All measures were created
from pre-existing scales. Attitude toward the computer desk was
assessed using five semantic differential pairs including: good-bad,
appealing-not appealing, unpleasant-pleasant, attractive-unattractive,
and boring-interesting. The scale was found to be reliable (α=.91).
Purchase intent was measured as the agreement or disagreement
with seven items dealing with the purchase decision and was also
found to be reliable (α=.86). Attitude toward Web sites using
3D product presentations was measured using the same attitude
scale used for the desk, however subjects were asked to consider
how they felt about Web sites using 3D product representations.
Finally, subjects were asked to rate their agreement or disagreement
with two statements about future intentions to view 3D products
if available. These items were also measured on a seven-point
scale.
Before the hypotheses could be tested, several checks of the data
were performed to address potential confounds to the study. Given
that the variable of interest was novelty, subjects’ prior
experiences with 3D product presentations and virtual reality
in general were assessed. A person was classified as having significant
experience with 3D product presentations if their mean score on
the items assessed in the pretest were 5 or greater (on a 7 point
scale). A t-test between those with significant experience (x
= 5.94) and those with little experience (x
= 3.09) revealed them to be significantly different, t(15)=16.46,
p<.001. The 39 subjects reporting significant experience with
3D experiences were excluded from further data analysis.
A series of ANCOVAs was used to examine the relationship between the degree of novelty, the presence of attribute information, and the dependent variables of interest: Attitude toward the product, purchase intent, attitude toward Web sites using 3D product representations, and future intention to view 3D product presentations (Means are presented in Table 1). Believability was specified as covariate for each of the analyses to examine its relationship to the dependent variables. The first hypothesis concerned attitudes toward products presented in a 3D environment. It was believed that the novelty of 3D product presentations would be positively related to attitudes toward those experiences. Results from the first ANCOVA failed to show a significant interaction between novelty and information, nor a main effect for those for whom the 3D desk was a novel experience (x = 5.13) and those for whom the 3D desk was not novel (x = 4.94), p>.05. However, there was a significant difference between the attitude toward the product when information was present (x = 5.29) or not (x = 4.77), F(1,94)=5.71, p<.05. Believability was also significantly related to subjects attitudes about the desk, F(1,94)=19.91, p<.001.
Table 1. Novelty and Attribute Information on Attitude Toward the Desk
The second hypothesis was similar to the first,
but examined subjects’ purchase intentions when
faced with 3D product presentations. It was believed that the
novelty of 3D product presentations would be positively related
to intentions to purchase the product. Results from the second
ANCOVA also failed to show a significant interaction between
novelty and information, nor a main effect for the presence
of attribute information with the 3D product presentation. However,
the purchase intention of those for whom the desk was a novel
presentation (x
= 4.76) was greater than for those who had been exposed to multiple
3D product presentations (x
= 4.26), F(1,94)=5.48, p<.05. Believability was
again significantly related to subjects’ willingness to
purchase the desk, F(1,94)=4.11, p<.05.
The third hypothesis concerned attitudes towards Web sites that provide 3D product presentations. An inverse relationship
was expected between the novelty of a 3D product presentation
and subjects’ attitudes toward Web sites’ using
such a tactic. ANCOVA was again conducted, revealing a significant
interaction between novelty and the presence of attribute information
on attitudes toward Web sites that present 3D products, F(1,94)=5.61,
p<.05. Figure 2 shows that subjects who are familiar with
3D product presentations and are also given attribute information
(x = 6.37),
and subjects who are not familiar with 3D product presentations
and are not given attribute information (x
= 6.35) have more positive attitudes
than those for whom 3D product presentations are novel (x
= 5.81). Again, believability was significant in the model (F(1,94)=18.98,
p<.05), suggesting that 3D product presentations are only
effective to the extent that they are true to life.
The last analysis examined the extent to which subjects were
likely to view 3D product presentations in the future. A final
ANCOVA showed no significant interaction or main effects. All
conditions reported that further viewing of 3D product presentations
was extremely likely (x
= 6.20). Only the degree to which the product was believable
was significant in the model, F(1,94)=5.25, p<.05.
The current study provides mixed
results concerning the relationship between novelty and the effectiveness
of 3D product presentations. First, no effect of novelty was seen
in product attitude formation. That is, subjects reported similar
positive attitudes toward the desk after a single exposure to
3D product presentations or after five exposures. However, the
enjoyment of the experience as reflected in the attitude toward
the computer desk seems to have been reduced by the presence of
attribute information. When copy provided additional information,
the subjects reported a significantly less positive attitude toward
the desk, than when the product was presented alone. This at first
may seem counter-intuitive, however the literature on information
overload may provide some insight. As the number stimuli demanding
attention increases, each individual source has less impact on
attitude formation (Baron 1986, March 1994). This finding highlights
the experiential nature of 3D product presentations and suggests
that subjects may have enjoyed the experience to a greater degree
when they were not reminded of the need to consider utilitarian
features of the product.
In contrast, subjects reported attribute information as equally
important when thinking about a purchase. Here only novelty was
a key determinant in whether or not consumers intended on purchasing
the computer desk. When 3D product presentations were novel, subjects
were more likely to purchase than when the presentations were
not novel. This finding provides evidence for the existence of
a novelty effect concerning purchase intention. However, the data
was particularly surprising given the well documented research
showing that purchase intentions are derived from attitudes (Brown and Stayman 1992). Further research is needed to explore
this finding and determine why novelty impacted intention to purchase,
but not attitude formation in this study.
Further evidence for the novelty effect was shown when examining
the data on presenting product attribute information and attitudes
toward Web sites using 3D product presentations in general. Subjects
for whom the 3D product presentations were not novel had more
positive attitudes towards Web sites using 3D than those for whom
3D presentations were novel, but only when given accompanying
product information.. Along with the previous findings, this result
indicates that the focus of the subjects shifted depending upon
whether they were familiar with the 3D product presentations or
not. When familiar, subjects prefer the 3D representation along
with attribute information to aid in decision making. In contrast,
when novel, subjects’ attitudes toward Web sites with 3D
products was attenuated by the utilitarian reminder of product
information. Or as Cox and Locander (1987) found, it could be
possible that the novel nature if the stimulus overcame or blocked
the motivation to process attribute information.
Regardless, all subjects reported that in the future they were
likely to view products in 3D if available. This finding indicates
that they enjoyed the experience equally well. Therefore, novelty
may not pose a great risk to companies using these technologies.
If consumers are attracted to viewing 3D products, then both companies
and consumers will benefit. However, as the use of 3D product
presentations becomes more common, it will become increasingly
important for online stores to develop the believability of the
product presentations. The results of the study found believability
to co-vary with each dependent measure. Therefore, as consumers
adopt 3D technologies, they will look at the product features
with increased scrutiny, and will demand more realistic visuals
and images.
In this paper, we investigated
a tactic that is currently being tested for its usefulness as
a sales tool in online environments. Companies such as Office
Depot, Sony, Lands’ End, and The Sharper Image Stores are
exploring this technology for its impact on sales. To the degree
that consumers are persuaded by rotating and virtually experiencing
new products, these product displays may eventually be incorporated
into the ubiquitous banner ad. However, as the effectiveness of
3D product presentations are tested, it must be realized that
a portion of the benefit derived from their use can be explained
by a novelty effect. Therefore, caution must be taken in attributing
changes in traditional measures of advertising effectiveness to
consumers’ interactions with 3D products. Some of the
effects appear to be due to novelty, but the findings are equivocal
and must be further examined.
The conceptualization of novelty in terms of the number of exposures
to 3D product presentations in one moment in time could have led
to the mixed results. One limitation of this study therefore,
is in not considering the effect of time. Conceptualization of
familiarity with 3D product presentations based on length of time
may lead to a deeper understanding of novelty. This is particularly
true as the novelty effect and its wearing out should be a continuous
phenomenon that occurs over a longer period of time. Given the
present discrepant findings, novelty could be examined over a
greater number of experiences and a longer period of time. In
fact, it would be interesting to identify the point at which habituation
and tedium occur in response to 3D products online.
Future research should also examine the degree to which highly
experiential 3D product simulations decrease the level of information
processing. To the degree that consumers are enjoying the product
experience, they should form positive attitudes under conditions
of low involvement. However given the directed nature of most
product searches, product interactions online are likely to be
highly cognitive experiences where consumers are likely to make
rational purchase decisions. It would be valuable to understand
the relationships of involvement, novelty and attitude formation.
A third area worthy of examination is the impact of experiential
product selling on impulse purchasing online. To the degree that
3D product presentations create enjoyable experiences for consumers,
they may be more willing to engage in impulse purchases. Virtual
product trial may help consumers reduce feelings of perceived
risk and limit the formation of counter-arguments when 3D product
presentations are novel. If consumers focus on the experiential
aspects of the product display, they may be willing to buy on
impulse. When confronted with a non-novel situation, the experience
may not be enough to encourage immediate purchase.
Novelty and virtual product experiences online are ripe areas for further research. From the current study, it is apparent that 3D product presentations can be beneficial for consumers and advertisers. However to the extent that effectiveness can be explained by novelty, online retailers must be ready to provide repeat consumers with the necessary information to make their decisions. 3D product presentations provide a unique form of information as virtual experience. Copy provides additional product attribute information. The combined effect of both types of information should help consumers make better decisions. However, even if waning novelty signals less effective 3D product presentations, these presentations should remain another tool that consumers use to help make their decisions. However, it will still fall to advertisers to vary the presentations of their messages to attract attention and maintain interest, beyond the novelty of interacting with 3D product presentations online.
Figure 1. Schematic Diagram of the Habituation-Tedium Theory (Tellis 1997)
|
Figure 2. Interaction of Novelty and Level of Information
Aaker, David. A., and Douglas M. Stayman (1989). What Mediates Emotional Responses to Advertising?: The Case of Warmth,In Patricia Cafferata and Alice Tybout (Eds.), Cognitive and Affective Responses to Advertising. Lexington, MA: Heath and Company.
Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict Theory: Progress and Problems, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 1-39.
Beltramini, Richard (1982). Advertising Perceived Believability Scale, In D. R. Corrigan, F. B. Kraft and R. H. Ross (Eds.), Proceedings of the Southwestern Marketing Association. Wichita, KS: Wichita State University, 1-3.
Bettman, James R., E. J. Johnson, and John W. Payne (1991). Consumer Decision Making, In Thomas S. Robertson and Harold H. Kassarjian (Eds.), Handbook of Consumer Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 50-84.
Briggs, R. and N. Hollis (1997). Advertising On The Web: Is There Response Before Click-through?, Journal of Advertising Research, 37(2), 33-35.
Brown, Steven P. and Douglas M. Stayman (1992). Antecedents and Consequences of Attitude Toward the Ad: A Meta-Analysis, Journal of Consumer Research, 19(1), 34-52.
Carver, Charles S. and Michael F. Scheier (1981). Attention and Self-regulation: A Control Theory Approach to Human Behavior. New York: Springer Press.
Cho, Chang-Hoan (1999). How Advertising Works on the WWW: Modified Elaboration Likelihood Model, Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 21 (Spring) 33-50.
Chen Ang and Paul W. Darst (2001). Situational Interest in Physical Education: A Function of Learning Task Design, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 72(2), 150-164.
Cox, Dena Salvages and William B. Locander (1987). Product Novelty: Does It Moderate the Relationship Between Ad Attitudes and Brand Attitudes? Journal of Advertising, 16(3), 39-45.
Deci, Edward L. (1992). The Relation of Interest to the Motivation of Behavior: A Self-determination Theory Perspective. In K. Ann Renninger, Suzanne Hidi, Andreas Krapp (Eds.), The Role of Interest in Learning and Development. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA, 43-69.
Edwards, Steven M. and Carrie La Ferle (2001). Role-taking and Empathic Processing in a Computer-Based Environment, presented at the winter conference of the Society for Consumer Psychology. Abstract published in the 2001 proceedings, 17.
Fahy, F. L., I. P. Riches, M.W. Brown (1993). Neuronal Activity Related to Visual Recognition Memory: Long-term Memory and the Encoding of Recency and Familiarity Information in the Primate Anterior and Medial Inferior Temporal and Rhinal Cortex, Experimental Brain Research, 96, 457-472.
Fitzgerald, Kathryn (1999). Beyond Apples and Oranges: Using Analogies to Catalyze New Product Adoption, Dissertation Abstracts International, 59(9A), 3550.
Gibson, James J. (1996). The
Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Haugtvedt,
Curtis P., David W. Schumann and Wendy L. Schneier (1994). Advertising
Repetition and Variation Strategies: Implications for Understanding
Attitude Strength, Journal
of Consumer Research, 21(1), 176-190.
Heeter, Carrie (2000). Interactivity in the Context of Designed Experience, Journal of Interactive Advertising, 1(1), [online] http://www.jiad.org.
Hoch, Stephen J. and John Deighton
(1989). Managing What Consumers Learn From Experience, Journal
of Marketing, 53(April), 1-20.
Hoffman, Martin L.
(1986). Affect, Cognition and Motivation, in Richard M. Serpentine and E. Tory Higgins (Eds.), Handbook
of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior.
New York: Guilford Press, 244-280.
Hoffman, Donna L. and Thomas P. Novak (1996). Marketing in Hypermedia Computer-Based Environments: Conceptual Foundations, Journal of Marketing, 60(July), 50-68.
Jolibert, Alain and Gary Baumgartner (1997). Values, Motivation, and Personal Goals: Revisited, Psychology and Marketing, 14(7), 675-688.
Jupiter Communications Inc. (1999). Defining the Internet Shopper: The Jupiter/NFO Consumer Survey Vol. 1, Attitudes, Objectives and Behavior, [online] Available: http://www.jpu.com/store/studies/jup_nfol/
Kover, Arthur J. and William L. James (1993). When do Advertising ‘Power Words’ Work? An Examination of Congruence and Satiation, Journal of Advertising Research, 33(4), 32-39.
Li, Lin, Earl K. Miller and Robert Desimone (1993). The Representation of Stimulus Familiarity in Anterior Inferior Temporal Cortex, Journal of Neurophysiology, 69, 1918-1929.
Li, Hairong, Terry M. Daugherty and Frank Biocca (2001a). Feeling the Presence of Products: Consumer Learning from Virtual Experience, Presented at the American Marketing Association, Winter Educators Conference, February 16-19, in Scottsdale, AZ.
___________ (2001b). Characteristics of Virtual Experience in E-Commerce: A Protocol Analysis, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 15(3), 13-30.
MacInnis, Deborah J. and Bernard J. Jaworski (1989). Information Processing From Advertisements: Toward An Integrative Framework, Journal of Marketing, 53(October), 1-23.
MacInnis, Deborah J., Christine Moorman, and Bernard J. Jaworski (1991) Enhancing and Measuring Consumers’ Motivation, Opportunity, and Ability to Process Brand Information From Ads, Journal of Marketing, 55(October), 32-53.
March, J. G. (1994). A
Primer on Decision Making: How Decisions Happen. New York: The
Free Press.
Millar, Murray G. and Karen U.
Millar. (1996). The Effects of Direct and Indirect Experience
on Affective and Cognitive Response and the Attitude-Behavior
Relation, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32,
561-579.
Meeker, Mary (1997). The Internet Advertising Report, New York, NY: Harper Business.
Nash, Sharon (2000). Online Catalogs Will Come to Life and You’ll View Merchandise From Every Side, PC Magazine, January 2000. [online] http://www.zdnet.com/pcmag/stories/trends/0,7607,2418240,00.html
Novak, Thomas P., Donna L. Hoffman and Yiu-Fai Yung (2000), “Measuring the Customer Experience in Online Environments: A Structural Modeling Approach,” working paper.
Oliver, Richard L., Thomas S. Robertson and Deborah J. Mitchell (1993). Imaging and Analyzing in Response to New Product Advertising, Journal of Advertising, 22(4), 35-50.
Pavlou, Paul A. and David W. Stewart (2000). Measuring the Effects and Effectiveness of Interactive Advertising: A Research Agenda, Journal of Interactive Advertising, 1(1). [online] http://www.jiad.org.
Pfau, Michael, Roxanne Parrott and Bridget Lindquist (1992). An Expectancy Theory Explanation of the Effectiveness of Political Attack Television Spots: A Case Study, Journal of Applied Communication Research, 20(3), 235-253.
Raju, P. S. (1980). Optimum Stimulation Level: Its Relationship to Personality, Demographics, and Exploratory Behavior, Journal of Consumer Research, 7(3), 272.
Reeve, Johnmarshall (1996). Motivating Others: Nurturing Inner Motivational Resources. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Riches, I. P., F. A. Wilson and Malcolm W. Brown (1991). The Effects of Sensual Stimulation and Memory on Neurons of the Hippocampal Formation and the Neighboring Parahippocampal Gyrus and Inferior Temperal Cortex of the Primate, Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 1763-1771.
Steenkamp, Jan-Benedict E. M. and Hans Baumgartner (1992). The Role of Optimum Stimulation Level in Exploratory Consumer Behavior, Journal of Consumer Research, 19(December), 434-448.
Spielberger, Charles, D. and Laura M. Starr (1994). Curiosity and Exploratory Behavior, in Harold F. O’Neil Jr. and Michael Drillings (Eds.), Motivation: Theory and Research. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA, 221-243.
Tellis, Gerard J. (1997). Effective Frequency: One Exposure or Three Factors? Journal of Advertising Research, 37(4), 75-80.
Wilson, F.A. and E. T. Rolls (1993). The Effects of Stimulus Novelty and Familiarity on Neuronal Activity in the Amygdala of Monkeys Performing Recognition Memory Tasks, Experimental Brain Research, 9, 367-382.
Steven Edwards is Assistant Professor in the Department of Advertising at Michigan State University. Edwards’ work explores persuasive communication in new media environments. He is currently examining forced exposure situations online and consumer avoidance of advertising. Other areas of interest include human perception and cognition in 3-D virtual environments, specifically related to e-commerce applications. Harshavardhan Gangadharbatla is a graduate student in the Department of Advertising at Michigan State University working on research involving internet advertising.