This article reviews the empirical literature on interactivity, primarily studies based on experimental designs, and concludes that two conceptualizations of interactivity are beginning to dominate: the functional and the perceptual. Suggestions concerning future experiments with interactivity are offered.
As many have noted, there is little consistency in the communication research literature concerning the proper conceptualization of interactivity. This has led to the unfortunate situation of scholars sometimes reaching contradictory conclusions, not because their findings are necessarily at odds, but because the definitions of key terms are not the same. Although this is bound to occur with emerging concepts, advancement in theory is enhanced when communities of scholars reach consensus on conceptual building blocks (Shoemaker, Tankard, and Lasorsa 2004).
While a universal definition for interactivity has yet to be attained, there is, interestingly, some agreement among experimentalists on how to manipulate it, usually by varying the quantity and quality of channel features, most typically on the Web. This paper starts on this common ground, reviewing (primarily) the empirical literature, and then arguing that, while one study can push us only so far towards a workable definition, collectively the results of these studies help guide us in the proper direction.
Although the argument will take some time to advance, the conclusions are these: there are two important variants of interactivity, and these should be examined independently as well as in concert. A number of recommendations for interactivity research are offered in the final section as well.
From the beginning, Web researchers focused attention on interactivity (Deighton 1996; Hoffman and Novak 1996; Rafaeli and Sudweeks 1997). A common approach involved an investigation of features of the new medium that made it different from traditional media. Initial empirical work focused on categorizing these features, often through content analyses. Ha and James (1998) proposed five dimensions of interactivity where each dimension was tied to a corresponding Web structure. Playfulness was indicated by the presence of games or FAQ sections on a site; choice by the customizability of the site (color, language, download speeds); connectedness by the use of hyperlinks; information collection by the presence of registration forms and hit counters; and reciprocal communication by the presence of e-mail links, phone numbers, chat sessions with site managers and order placement mechanisms.
Ghose and Dou (1998) listed 23 site characteristics and analyzed 101 corporate sites to find out which interactive features were the most prevalent and could best predict a high listing on a search engine. McMillan (1999) and Massey and Levy (1999) applied Heeter’s (1989) dimensions of interactivity to analyses of websites using site features as indicators of interactivity. Bucy et al. (1999) coded online surveys, e-mail links, text links, graphic links, addresses, phone number and site counters as “interactive features” of dot-com websites. Aikat (2000) examined 264 Fortune 500 company websites for indicators of interactivity including dynamic content, interactive content, search features and intelligent agents.
Many early conceptualizations of interactivity were similarly “channel” driven. Some researchers followed Steuer’s lead (1992). Steuer’s important work categorized media by their ability to support telepresence, one dimension of which he labeled as interactivity. To define interactivity, Steuer referred to the “malleability of a medium’s form and content” (p. 85). Other researchers have adopted this approach and defined interactivity as a “characteristic of a medium” (Choi, Miracle, and Biocca 2001; Lombard and Snyder-Duch 2001; Roehm and Haugtvedt 1999).
But even while Steuer and others focused their attention on the characteristics of media, few argued that interactivity existed in technologies absent their use by people. Steuer’s longer definition makes this apparent: “Interactivity is the extent to which users can participate in modifying the form and content of a mediated environment in real time” (p. 84). Similarly, Roehm and Haugtvedt noted that one aspect of interactivity is that it allows “consumers to participate in the formation of the content of the communication and its presentation” (1999, p. 32). Both Choi, Miracle, and Biocca (2001) and Lombard and Snyder-Duch (2001) added the phrase “in which a user can influence the form and/or content” to their categorization of interactivity as a facet of channel structure. Pavlou and Stewart (2000) went one step further and labeled interactivity a characteristic not of media but of consumers.
The interactive advertising model proposed by Rodgers and Thorson (2000) incorporates both the structural perspective and functional view, because, as they point out “structure alone cannot explain what drives individuals to enter cyberspace, and how they react to the physical features of Internet ads once the cyber journey has begun.” People are the agents of interactivity.
Cho and Leckenby (1999) focused on the user in their definition of interactivity in advertising, “the degree to which a person actively engages in advertising processing by interacting with advertising messages and advertisers.” The researchers noted two primary locations of interactivity: human-human interaction and human-message interaction. They proceeded to treat interactivity as both a dependent variable where individual characteristics served as antecedents and as an independent variable with consequences for attitude change and purchase intention.
Although, as the following section will explore, many experimental researchers use site features as a way to manipulate interactivity, most are not claiming that channel characteristics alone are interactivity. The question then is, what precisely are researchers manipulating when they build experiment websites with varying amounts of interactive features? A review of experimental research suggests that two dominant conceptualizations of interactivity are being employed.
Multidisciplinary literature reviews have identified three primary conceptualizations of interactivity (Kiousis 2002; McMillan 2002). The first is the structural view discussed above. Although experimentalists have used web structures in their research designs, in most cases the conceptualizations offered center on the other two: process and perceptual.
Interactivity
as a Process of Message Exchange
Rafaeli (1988) is the most cited proponent of the interactivity-as-process
point of view. He called it a variable quality of communication
settings that referred to how reciprocal a particular exchange
was. His conceptualization of interactivity was the first applied
to Internet research. Ogan (1993) applied the work of Rafaeli
and Ball-Rokeach and Reardon (1988) to an examination of postings
to an electronic bulletin board. Specifically she examined messages
for evidence of the “exchange, associational, and debate
functions” identified by Ball-Rokeach and Reardon. All
are communication, not technology, related concepts.
Heeter (1989), whose original six-dimension construct for interactivity is widely cited, has more recently proposed a participant-centered conceptualization (Heeter 2000). Under this view, the user’s experiences with a particular technology define the concept, specifically: “actions the participant is capable of observing through one or more senses over whatever channels exist to connect the participant to the experience.” Interactivity, here, is what occurs on the channels, not the channels themselves or their characteristics. The technology affords the interactivity but does not define interactivity.
Interactivity-as-process has been examined, although less frequently, by researchers using experimental designs. Cho and Leckenby (1999) used the participant perspective in a study on the effectiveness of banner ads. Study participants were exposed to the ads using a forced exposure manipulation. The most direct operationalization for the interactivity-as-process conceptualization would be direct measurement of user interactions with the text. Cho and Leckenby acknowledged this but used an indirect method because it was more practical: a self-reported measure of intention to interact. Interactivity measured in this way was causally related to favorable attitude toward the brand and intention to purchase. Figure 1 below shows how this study fits with other interactivity experiments.
Figure 1: A Model for Interactivity Research with Exemplar Studies
Cho
and Leckenby (1999) conceptualized interactivity as a process,
specifically the degree to which a person interacted with the
ad. This conceptualization, and their operationalization, fit
the functional view of interactivity. Although their operationalization
is cognitive, it is not the same as a measure of the perception
of interactivity.
A similarly constructed experiment was conducted by Macias (2003).
She, too, proposed a process-oriented conceptualization: “interactivity
is the state or process of communicating, exchanging, obtaining
and/or modifying content and/or its form with or through a medium.”
Macias examined the role of interactivity on company websites
on comprehension and persuasion regarding company products.
Like the Cho and Leckenby (1999) study, participants were exposed
to web structures that were either high or low in interactive
potential. Similarly, participants’ use of these features
was not directly measured. Macias, however, employed a perceptual
measure as a manipulation check. Study participants were asked
how interactive they felt their website treatment to be. This
measure varied in the manner anticipated by the manipulation
of site structures.
Interactivity as a
Perceptual Variable
Although Macias (2003) did use a perceptual measure as a manipulation
check, some researchers have argued that interactivity is best
conceived as a perceptual variable (Bucy 2004). Bucy argues
that locating interactivity as a perceptual variable “routinizes
the concept and makes it a part of everyday media experience,”
and further, encourages “the concept’s theoretical
development by enabling empirical measurement through attitudinal
and emotional scales” (p. 377).
McMillan and Hwang (2002) noted that consumer perceptions are
of central importance in advertising research so the perception-based
approach to interactivity may be the most fruitful. They developed
an 18-item scale for the measurement of perceived interactivity
(MPI). This MPI scale was applied in a subsequent experiment
to compare the effects of structural and perceptual interactivity
(McMillan, Hwang, and Lee 2003). The researchers found some
evidence that the perception of interactivity was more closely
related to the dependent measure attitude toward the site than
was structural interactivity. Results of this experiment also
speak to the issue of functional, or process-related interactivity,
and that aspect will be taken up in a subsequent section.
Jee and Lee (2002) used a nine-item scale adapted from Wu (2000)
to measure perceived interactivity. They did something that
relatively few have done: look not only at the effects of (perceived)
interactivity but also at some of the causes (see Figure 1).
They found that need for cognition and web skills were predictors
of perceived interactivity. Further, they found that perceived
interactivity was positively associated with attitude toward
the site, which in turn was related to purchase intention.
Chung and Zhao (2004) also used an experimental design to examine
perceived interactivity and also included an individual characteristic,
motivation. The researchers found “a positive impact of
perceived interactivity on both attitude and memory” concerning
the ad, but the motivation manipulation had no significant consequence
for perceived interactivity.
Some initial conclusions can be drawn from the studies reviewed above. First, nearly every experiment concerning interactivity manipulates it by varying the amount and/or nature of interactive features available on websites or in banner ads. Second, although the manipulation of interactivity occurs in a similar fashion, differing conceptualizations are offered. Experimentalists are not proposing that interactivity is simply a characteristic of technology. Instead, all are concerned with human-message interaction and differ only in whether interactivity exists in the process of message exchange or in the minds of users. Third, there is often a mismatch between the conceptualization offered in a particular study and the operationalization used. Fourth, few studies have designed experiments that adequately measure both message exchange and perceived interactivity. The result can be lack of clarity in determining the causal mechanism responsible for changes in dependent variables (such as comprehension, attitude, and intention to purchase). A few studies have explored this issue and these are examined in greater detail here.
Lee
et al. (2004) compared what they called “objective”
site characteristics with users’ perceptions. Their findings
shed light on the problem under examination in this paper. Study
participants were asked to shop at three computer web stores
which had been content analyzed by the researchers. They had
coded each for the presence or absence of 88 interactive tools
(Stout, Villegas, and Kim 2001). While the content analysis
revealed no significant difference amongst the three on interactive
features, study participants during in-depth interviews rated
one site significantly more interactive than the others. The
authors offer a number of possible explanations for the discrepancy,
including the possibility that personal characteristics of users
might be interacting with other site characteristics, or that
some web features have more salience with the users than others.
Another possible explanation could be in how the sites were
navigated by the users and which of the interactive features
present were actually used. The sites could have roughly the
same number of interactive features but their unique design
may make it more or less likely that they were encountered (and
used) by study participants. This possibility is one the strongest
reasons for inclusion in experimental work of a detailed measure
of actual use by each study participant. According to the functional
view of interactivity, the mere presence or absence of certain
features matters most if it affects how the messages are consumed.
A similar issue is raised in the experiment of McMillan, Huang,
and Lee (2003). In that study, the authors concluded that “perceptual
variables seem to be stronger predictors of [attitude toward
the site] than structural variables” (p. 406). Involvement
was also found to be closely related to perceived interactivity.
But there was another finding that sheds light on the issue
of interactivity conceptualizations. The researchers found that
one of the sites with the fewest interactive features scored
well with participants on attitude toward the site. The reason,
according to the authors, may have been the presence of one
particular web feature, a virtual tour. They surmise that study
participants may have been much more likely to use it than other
interactive features such as reservations forms available on
some of the more interactive sites. Even though the site had
few interactive elements, one of the ones it did contain may
have been responsible for higher attitude toward the site scores.
A measurement of actual site use by study participants might
reveal the true causal mechanism.
The experiment by Chung and Zhao (2004) utilized measures of
both functional and perceptual interactivity which makes it
particularly relevant here (see Figure 1). The authors were
first interested in the role of involvement on functional interactivity.
They found that users with high product involvement were more
interactive with product-related content than those with low
involvement. Those with low product involvement also exhibited
interactive behavior but with content not related to the product.
In both cases, perceived interactivity (a five-item scale) was
related to functional interactivity regardless of involvement.
Further, the researchers found that perceived interactivity
resulted not from the presence of certain structures, but from
the interaction with them by users. This was measured by recording
every click of a study participant’s mouse. And perceived
interactivity was positively associated with a post-test of
product recall. Finally, the authors controlled for perceived
interactivity and level of involvement and found that clicking
behavior was still significantly related to product recall.
This final result they attributed to the collinearity of perceived
and functional interactivity.
These three studies highlight the importance of having separate
measures for functional and perceptual interactivity. A perception
of high interactivity can occur even when the structures necessary
for it do not seem to be present (McMillan, Huang, and Lee 2003).
Likewise, perception of interactivity can be low even when many
interactive features are available if, for whatever reason,
subjects are not using them. Because certain dependent variables
(attitude toward site is one) can be influenced by both the
perception of interactivity and by actual interaction with the
content, causal mechanisms are best revealed by designs where
each type of interactivity is measured.
The preceding analysis of interactivity literature, particularly in experimental research, reveals important trends. When it comes to conceptualizations of interactivity, most researchers are choosing between the functional view of interactivity and the perceptual view and are designing their studies accordingly. Regardless of which conception is picked, most are manipulating interactivity by varying the amount and nature of web structures. Conclusion one: Web structures are increasingly seen as antecedents or necessary conditions for interactivity but not as interactivity itself, leaving the other two conceptualizations of interactivity as the dominant perspectives.
Some researchers offer a functional conceptualization of interactivity but for the purposes of a particular experiment, measure perceived interactivity instead. The opposite is also possible. In some cases, it’s unclear which definition is operating. For example, when a researcher creates high and low interactivity conditions by varying web structures and then notes a significant effect on a dependent variable, we are often left wondering whether study participants actually exhibited differential interactive behavior (functional view) and this led to the effect, or if participants simply perceived one site to be more interactive (perceptual view) and that led to the effect. Of course, the evidence reviewed above suggests that each effect could be occurring. Conclusion Two: Functional interactivity and perceived interactivity are independent, although certainly related concepts.
Recommendation
One: More experiments are needed that measure both functional
interactivity and perceived interactivity. If functional interactivity
and perceptual interactivity are unique concepts, it is important
to determine how and when the two are causally related and how
and when they are not. For a given dependent variable, it is
also important to determine which of the two is really responsible
for any association. Without careful measures for each, causation
cannot be isolated, and that is one of the primary purposes
of experimental design. Liu and Shrum (2002) had it right when
they concluded, “one important explanation for the disparate
results regarding interactivity effects can be traced directly
to how the construct is defined and operationalized” (p.
63).
Recommendation Two: More experiments need to use non-forced
exposure methods. Most of the experimental studies in advertising
force exposure to treatments and then measure dependent variables
via questionnaire. This is understandable as a starting point
for the investigation of interactivity. But the true nature
of a user’s interactions with content may be changed by
the requirement to view unwanted content. For example, a user
may engage in heavy amounts of interactive behavior driven more
by exploration than by interest in the “interactions”
themselves. Depending on the dependent measures, causal relationships
can be distorted.
Recommendation Three: More focus is needed on cognitive processing.
What are users thinking when they engage in interactive behavior?
Are they actively engaged with the content or is something else
going on? Some research has been done in this area (Light and
Wakeman 2001; Tremayne and Dunwoody 2001) but more is warranted.
Tremayne and Dunwoody (2001) found that some clicking behavior
that would otherwise be classified as functional interactivity
was really an attempt at orientation by users. Light and Wakeman
(2001) examined thought processes during the inputting of text
by users, a higher order of interaction.
Recommendation Four: More focus is needed on user traits as
antecedents. What causes a person to engage in interactive behavior
when others avoid it? Some researchers have raised this issue
(Chung and Zhao 2004; Heeter 2000; Jee and Lee 2002; Pavlou
and Stewart 2000) but there is still more work to be done here.
When it comes to interactivity, having the right web structures
is important, but so is knowing your audience.
Recommendation Five: Expand the range of functional interactivity.
Most of the interactive structures and interaction behaviors
measured in interactivity research so far involve hypertext
links. This is a rather narrow range of interactivity. There
are many other types of interactive advertising (Faber, Lee,
and Nan 2004). What happens when a user gets involved in a chat
room or a discussion board at a company website? What happens
when the consumer can customize a product to their preferences
and view the finished product online? What happens when users
have input in the ad campaigns chosen by corporations? The depth
and complexity of user interaction with content and the consequences
of this are areas of interactivity research that are largely
unexplored.
Finally, whether researchers follow the functional or perceptual
conceptualization of interactivity, the audience for the web
is ever-changing. That means the expectations they bring to
a particular episode of web use are ever-changing (Lievrouw
2004). For researchers and practitioners this means attempts
to predict user behavior will always be a challenge.
Aikat,
Debashis (2000), “A New Medium for Organizational Communication:
Analyzing Web Content Characteristics of Fortune 500 Companies,”
The Electronic Journal of Communication, 10 (1 and
2).
Ball-Rokeach, Sandra J. and Kathleen K. Reardon (1988), “Monologue,
Dialogue, and Telelog,” in Advancing Communication
Science: Merging Mass and Interpersonal Processes, R. P.
Hawkins, J. M. Wiemann, and S. Pingree, Eds. Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Bucy, Erik P. (2004), “Interactivity in Society: Locating
an Elusive Concept,” Information Society, 20,
373-83.
—, Annie Lang, Robert F. Potter, and Maria
Elizabeth Grabe (1999), “Formal Features of Cyberspace:
Relationships between Web Page Complexity and Site Traffic,”
Journal of the American Society for Information Science,
50 (13), 1246-1256.
Cho, Chang-Hoan and John D. Leckenby (1999), “Interactivity
as a Measure of Advertising Effectiveness: Antecedents and Consequences
of Interactivity in Web Advertising,” in Proceedings
of the 1999 Conference of the American Academy of Advertising,
Pullman, Washington: American Academy of Advertising.
Choi, Yung Kyun, Gordon E. Miracle, and Frank Biocca (2001),
“The Effects of Anthropomorphic Agents on Advertising Effectiveness
and the Mediating Role of Presence,” Journal of Interactive
Advertising, 2 (1), <http://www.jiad.org/vol2/no1/choi/index.html >
Chung, Hwiman and Xinshu Zhao (2004), “Effects of Perceived
Interactivity on Web Site Preference and Memory: Role of Personal
Motivation,” Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,
10 (1).
Deighton, John (1996), “The Future of Interactive Marketing,”
Harvard Business Review, 74 (6), 151-161.
Faber,
Ronald J., Mira Lee, and Xiaoli Nan (2004), “Advertising
and the Consumer Information Environment Online,” American
Behavioral Scientist, 48 (4), 447-466.
Ghose, Sanjoy and Wenyu Dou (1998), “Interactive Functions
and their Impacts on the Appeal of Internet Presence Sites,”
Journal of Advertising Research, 38 (2), 29-43.
Ha, Louisa and E. Lincoln James (1998), “Interactivity
Reexamined: A Baseline Analysis of Early Business Web Sites,”
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 42
(4), 457-474.
Heeter, Carrie (1989), “Implications of New Interactive
Technologies for Conceptualizing Communication,” in Media Use in the Information Age: Emerging Patterns of Adoption
and Consumer Use, J. L. Salvaggio and J. Bryant, Eds.,
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
— (2000), “Interactivity in the Context
of Designed Experiences,” Journal of Interactive Advertising,
1 (1),<http://www.jiad.org/vol1/no1/heeter/index.html>.
Hoffman, Donna L. and Thomas P. Novak (1996), “Marketing
in Hypermedia Computer-Mediated Environments: Conceptual Foundations,”
Journal of Marketing, 60 (3), 50-68.
Jee, Joonhyung and Wei-Na Lee (2002), “Antecedents and
Consequences of Perceived Interactivity: An Exploratory Study,”
Journal of Interactive Advertising, 3 (1), <http://www.jiad.org/vol3/no1/jee/index.htm>.
Kiousis, Spiro (2002), “Interactivity: A Concept Explication,”
New Media & Society, 4 (3), 355-383.
Lee, Se-Jin, Wei-Na Lee, Hyojin Kim, and Patricia Stout (2004),
“A Comparison of Objective Characteristics and User Perception
of Web Sites,” Journal of Interactive Advertising,
4 (2), <http://www.jiad.org/vol4/no2/lee/index.htm>.
Lievrouw, Leah (2004), “What’s Changed about New Media?
Introduction to the Fifth Anniversary Issue of New Media and
Society,” New Media & Society, 6 (1), 9-15.
Light, Ann and Ian Wakeman (2001), “Beyond the Interface:
Users’ Perceptions of Interaction and Audience on Websites,”
Interacting with Computers, 13, 325-351.
Liu, Yuping and L. J. Shrum (2002), “What is Interactivity
and is it Always such a Good Thing? Implications of Definition,
Person, and Situation for the Influence of Interactivity on
Advertising Effectiveness,” Journal of Advertising,
31 (4), 53-64.
Lombard, Matthew and Jennifer Snyder-Duch (2001), “Interactive
Advertising and Presence: A Framework,” Journal of
Interactive Advertising, 1 (2), <http://www.jiad.org/vol1/no2/lombard/index.html>.
Macias, Wendy (2003), “A Preliminary Structural Equation
Model of Comprehension and Persuasion of Interactive Brand Web
Sites,” Journal of Interactive Advertising, 3
(2), <http://www.jiad.org/vol3/no2/macias/index.htm>.
Massey, Brian L. and Mark R. Levy (1999), “Interactivity,
Online Journalism and English-language Web Newspapers in Asia,”
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 76 (1),
138-151.
McMillan, Sally J. (1999), “Health Communication and the
Internet: Relations between Interactive Characteristics of the
Medium and Site Creators, Content, and Purpose,” Health
Communication, 11 (4), 375-390.
— (2002), “Exploring Models of Interactivity
from Multiple Research Traditions: Users, Documents, and Systems,”
in The Handbook of New Media: Social Shaping and Consequences
of ICTs, Leah Lievrouw and Sonia Livingstone, Eds., Sage
Publications.
— and Jang-Sun Hwang (2002), “Measures
of Perceived Interactivity: An Exploration of the Role of Direction
of Communication, User Control, and Time in Shaping Perceptions
of Interactivity,” Journal of Advertising, 31
(3), 29-42.
—, —, and Guiohk Lee
(2003), “Effects of Structural and Perceptual Factors,”
Journal of Advertising Research, December, 400-409.
Ogan, Christine (1993), “Listserver Communication during
the Gulf War: What kind of Medium is the Electronic Bulletin
Board?,” Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media,
37, 177-196.
Pavlou, Paul A. and David W. Stewart (2000), “Measuring
the Effects and Effectiveness of Interactive Advertising: A
Research Agenda,” Journal of Interactive Advertising,
1 (1), <http://www.jiad.org/vol1/no1/pavlou/index.html>.
Rafaeli, Sheizaf (1988), “Interactivity: From New Media
to Communication,” in Advancing Communication Science:
Merging Mass and Interpersonal Processes, R. P. Hawkins, J.
M. Wiemann, and S. Pingree, Eds., Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
— and Fay Sudweeks (1997), “Networked
Interactivity,” Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,
2 (4).
Rodgers, Shelly and Esther Thorson (2000), “The Interactive
Advertising Model: How Users Perceive and Process Online Ads,”
Journal of Interactive Advertising, 1 (1), <http://www.jiad.org/vol1/no1/rodgers/index.html>.
Roehm, Harper A. and Curtis P. Haugtvedt (1999), “Understanding
Interactivity of Cyberspace Advertising,” in Advertising
and the World Wide Web, David W. Schumann and Esther Thorson,
eds., Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Shoemaker, Pamela J., James William Tankard, Jr., and Dominic
L. Lasorsa (2004), How to Build Social Science Theories, Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Steuer, Jonathan (1992), “Defining Virtual Reality: Dimensions
Determining Telepresence,” Journal of Communication,
42, 73-93.
Stout, Patricia A., Jorge Villegas, and Hyojin Kim (2001), “Linking
Learning and Interactivity on Health-Related Web Sites,”
Health Education Research, 16 (6), 721-733.
Tremayne, Mark and Sharon Dunwoody (2001), “Interactivity,
Information Processing, and Learning on the World Wide Web,”
Science Communication, 23 (2), 111-134.
Wu, Guohua (2000), “The Role of Perceived Interactivity
in Interactive Ad Processing,” unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Texas at Austin.
Mark Tremayne is an Assistant Professor in the School of Journalism at the University of Texas at Austin. His research focus is on web-based communication with special attention on interactivity and network theory. Email: [email protected]